How To Burn an ISO File to a USB Drive

How To Burn an ISO File to a USB Drive

so you have an ISO file that you want on a flash drive, or some other USB storage device. You also need to be able to bootfrom it. Sounds straightforward, right? Copy the file over and you’re done!
Unfortunately, it’s not that simple. Properly burning an ISO to USB is different than just copying the file. It’s even different than burning an ISO to a disc. Adding to the complexity is that you plan on booting from the USB drive once you’re done getting the ISO image on there.
Luckily, there’s a fantastic free tool that will handle all of this for you automatically. Continue on below for an easy tutorial on how to burn an ISO file to USB with the free Rufus program.

Difficulty: EasyTip: See Tip #1 at the bottom of the page if you want to burn an ISO file to a USB drive but you don’t need to boot from it when done. That process is a bit different… and easier!
Note: I should mention here that you’re never technically “burning” anything to a USB drive since there are no lasers or similar technology involved. This term has just been carried over from the common practice of burning an ISO image to an optical disc.
Time Required: “Burning” an ISO image file to a USB device, like a flash drive, usually takes less than 20 minutes but the total time depends a lot on the size of the ISO file.

How To Burn an ISO File to a USB Drive

UPDATE: This process works to burn the Technical Preview of Windows 10 to USB!
  1. Download Rufus, a free tool that will correctly prepare the USB drive, automaticallyextract the contents of the ISO file you have, and properly copy the files contained within it to your USB device, including any files in the ISO needed to make it bootable.

    Rufus is a portable program (does not install), works on Windows 8, 7, Vista, and XP, and will “burn” an ISO image file to any type of USB storage device you happen to have.

    Note: If you’d prefer to use a different ISO-to-USB tool, see Tip #3 at the bottom of the page. Of course if you do choose another program, you won’t be able to follow the instructions I’ve written here because they pertain specifically to Rufus.
     
  2. Double-click or double-tap on the rufus-1.4.12.exe file that you just downloaded. The Rufus program will start right away.

    As I mentioned earlier, Rufus is a portable program, meaning that it just runs as is. This is a big reason why I prefer this ISO-to-USB program over some of the other options out there.
     
  3. Insert the flash drive or other USB device you want to “burn” the ISO file to into your computer, assuming it’s not already plugged in.

    Important: Burning an ISO image to a USB drive will erase everything on the drive! Check that the USB drive is empty or that you have backed up any files you want to keep before continuing.
     
  4. From the Device drop-down at the top of the Rufus program screen, choose the USB storage device you want to burn the ISO file to.

    Tip: Rufus tells you the size of the USB device, as well as the drive letter and current free space on the drive. Use this information to double-check that you’re choosing the correct USB device, assuming you have more than one plugged in. Don’t worry about the free space indicated since you’ll be erasing the entire drive as part of this process.

    Note: If no USB drive is listed under Device, or you can’t find the drive you’re expecting to see, there may be an issue with the USB device you’re planning on using for the ISO image or Windows is having some sort of problem seeing the drive. Try another USB device and/or another USB port on your computer.
     
    Documentation made easy for .NET assemblies, COM, VB, VBA and DBs
    Retrieve more than 50 File formats, Find your Photo/File/Trash now!
  5. Leave the Partition scheme and target system typeFile system, and Cluster size alone unless you know what you’re doing or you’ve been advised to set any of those parameters to something else.

    For example, maybe a bootable tool you downloaded in ISO format advised on its website to make sure the file system is FAT32instead of NTFS if you’re burning to USB. In that case, make the File system change to FAT32 before continuing.
     
  6. You’re welcome to enter a custom volume label in the New volume label field, but leaving it at whatever the default happens to be, or even blank, shouldn’t have any impact on anything.

    Note: Most bootable ISO images include volume label information so you may see this change automatically during Step 11.
     
  7. Under Format Options, you’ll see a number of… yes, format options! You can leave them all in their default state but you’re welcome to select Check device for bad blocks if you have some concern that the flash drive or USB device you’re using may have an issue.
     
  8. Next to Create a bootable disk using, make sure ISO Image is selected and then tap or click on the CD/DVD icon.
     
  9. When the Open window appears, locate and then select the ISO image you want to burn to the flash drive.
     
  10. Once selected, tap or click on the Open button.
     
  11. Wait while Rufus inspects the ISO file you chose. This may take several seconds or may go by so quickly that you don’t even notice.

    Note: If you get an Unsupported ISO message, the ISO you chose is not supported for burning to USB by Rufus. In this case, try one of the other programs listed in Tip #3 below or check with the maker of the ISO image for more help getting their software to work from a USB drive.
     
  12. Tap or click on Start to start the “burning” of the ISO file to the USB device you chose.

    Note: If you get an Image is too big message, you’ll need to use a larger USB device or choose a smaller ISO image.
     
  13. Tap or click OK to the WARNING: ALL DATA ON DEVICE ‘XYZ’ WILL BE DESTROYED message that appears next.

    Important: Take this message seriously! Make sure the flash drive or other USB device is empty or that you’re fine with erasing everything on it.
     
  14. Wait while Rufus properly formats the USB drive so it’s bootable, and then copies all of the files contained in the ISO image you selected in Step 11 to the drive.

    Tip: The total time to do this depends very much on how large the ISO file you’re working with is. I’ve had small diagnostic tools (like the 18 MB ONTP&RE ISO) take under one minute, while larger images (like a 4 GB Windows 8.1 ISO) could take closer to 20 minutes. Your computer and USB hardware speeds are a big factor here as well.
     
  15. Once the status at the bottom of the Rufus program window says DONE, you can close Rufus and remove the USB drive.
     
  16. Boot from the USB drive now that it’s properly “burned” and then continue with whatever it is you’re using this bootable drive for.

    For example, if you’ve put a memory testing program on a flash drive, you can now boot from that flash drive and test your RAM with it. Same goes for bootable hard drive testing programspassword recovery toolsdata wipe programs, etc. See Tip #2 below for more on using this procedure for Windows installation ISO files.

    Tip: Booting from a USB drive is often as easy as plugging the drive in to any free USB port and then restarting your computer, but it can sometimes be much more complicated. Se my How to Boot From a USB Drive tutorial if you need help.

Tips & More Information

  1. Rufus, and related ISO-to-USB tools, are great when you need to get some sort of bootable program, or even an entire operating system, onto a USB drive. But what if you have an ISO image that you want to “burn” to a USB drive that isn’t intended to be booted from? An ISO of Microsoft Office comes to mind as a common example.

    In these cases, think of the ISO image you’re working with as just any other compressed format, like a ZIP file. Use your favorite file compression program – I’m a big fan of the free 7-Zip tool – to extract the contents of the ISO image directly onto the previously-formatted flash drive. That’s it!

    See this List of Free File Extractor Programs for some more free programs that work with ISO files in this way.
     
  2. You’re more than welcome to use the procedure I’ve outlined above with Rufus for Windows ISO images, like those you might have downloaded for Windows 8Windows 7, etc. However, there is a more “official” procedure that uses free software direct from Microsoft.

    I’ve written complete tutorials on these procedures, which also includes guidance on other aspects of installing Windows from a USB stick. See my How to Install Windows 8 From USB or How to Install Windows 7 From USB, depending on the version of Windows you’re installing.
     
  3. Some other free ISO-to-USB “burners” that I’ve used include UNetbootinISO to USB, and Universal USB Installer.
     
  4. Having trouble using Rufus or getting that ISO burned to USB? See my Get More Helppage for information on contacting me for more help.

Create a Bootable USB Flash Drive

Create a Bootable USB Flash Drive

The information below had taken from Create a Bootable USB Flash Drive
Applies To: Windows Server 2012 Essentials, Windows Server 2012 R2 Essentials
You can create a bootable USB flash drive to use to deploy Windows Server 2012 Essentials . The first step is to prepare the USB flash drive by using DiskPart, which is a command-line utility. For information about DiskPart, see DiskPart Command-Line Options.
For additional scenarios in which you may want to create or use a bootable USB flash drive, see the following topics:

To create a bootable USB flash drive

  1. Insert a USB flash drive into a running computer.
  2. Open a Command Prompt window as an administrator.
  3. Type diskpart.
  4. In the new command line window that opens, to determine the USB flash drive number or drive letter, at the command prompt, type list disk, and then click ENTER. The list disk command displays all the disks on the computer. Note the drive number or drive letter of the USB flash drive.
  5. At the command prompt, type select disk <X>, where X is the drive number or drive letter of the USB flash drive, and then click ENTER.
  6. Type clean, and the click ENTER. This command deletes all data from the USB flash drive.
  7. To create a new primary partition on the USB flash drive, type create part pri, and then click ENTER.
  8. To select the partition that you just created, type select part 1, and then click ENTER.
  9. To format the partition, type format fs=ntfs quick, and then click ENTER.

    Important
    If your server platform supports Unified Extensible Firmware Interface (UEFI), you should format the USB flash drive as FAT32 rather than as NTFS. To format the partition as FAT32, type format fs=fat32 quick, and then click ENTER.


  10. Type active, and then click ENTER.
  11. Type exit, and then click ENTER.
  12. When you finish preparing your custom image, save it to the root of the USB flash drive.

See Also

Image Writer for Windows

Win32 Disk Imager | SourceForge.net 

This tool is used for writing images to USB sticks or SD/CF cards on Windows.

This program was initially written to fill a need for Ubuntu Jaunty (9.04) Netbook Remix. Recently, it has grown in popularity for other project developers, namely Arm development projects (Android, Ubuntu on Arm, etc). Anyone is free to branch and modify this program. Patches are always welcome.


This project is currently being migrated to Sourceforge. Please be patient.
Downloads are now on Sourceforge only.
Kali Linux Live USB Install

Kali Linux Live USB Install

Booting and installing Kali from a USB stick is our favorite and fastest method of getting up and running. In order to do this, we first need to image the Kali ISO on a USB drive.

Preparing for the USB copy

  1. Download Kali linux.
  2. If running Windows, download Win32 Disk Imager.
  3. No special software is needed for a *nix OS.
  4. A USB Key (at least 2GB capacity for mini, 4GB for regular).

Kali Linux Live USB Install Procedure

Imaging Kali on a Windows Machine

  1. Plug your USB stick into your Windows USB port and launch the Win32 Disk Imager software
  2. Choose the Kali Linux ISO file to be imaged and verify that the USB drive to be overwritten is the correct one.
  3. kali-usb-install-windows
  4. Once the imaging is complete, safely eject the USB drive from the Windows machine. You can now use the USB device to boot into Kali Linux.

Imaging Kali on a Linux Machine

Creating a bootable Kali Linux USB key in a Linux environment is easy. Once you’ve downloaded your Kali ISO file, you can use dd to copy it over to your USB stick as follows (make sure you’re root first):
WARNING. Although the process of imaging Kali on a USB stick is very easy, you can just as easily destroy arbitrary partitions with dd if you do not understand what you are doing. Consider yourself warned.
  1. Plug in your USB device to your Linux computer’s USB port.
  2. Verify the device path of your USB storage with dmesg.
  3. Proceed to (carefully!) image the Kali ISO file on the USB device:
 dd if=kali.iso of=/dev/sdb bs=512k
That’s it, really! You can now boot into a Kali Live / Installer environment using the USB device.

Imaging Kali on a OSX Machine

Creating a bootable Kali Linux USB key in an OSX environment is similar to the Linux method. Once you’ve downloaded your chosen Kali ISO file, you can use dd to copy it over to your USB stick as follow:
WARNING. Although the process of imaging Kali on a USB stick is very easy, you can just as easily destroy arbitrary partitions with dd if you do not understand what you are doing. Consider yourself warned.
  1. Plug in your USB device to your Apple computer’s USB port.
  2. Verify the device path of your USB storage with diskutil list.
  3. Unmount the drive (note, in this example, the USB stick is /dev/disk2):
  4. diskutil unmount /dev/disk2
  5. Proceed to (carefully!) image the Kali ISO file on the USB device:
  6. sudo dd if=kali-linux-*.iso of=/dev/disk2 bs=1m
    Note: Increasing the blocksize (bs) will speed up the write progress, but increase the chances of creating a bad USB stick.
The time to dd the image across will depend on the speed of the system used, USB stick, and USB port.
And that’s it! You can now boot into a Kali Live / Installer environment using the USB device.
If you choose to boot on an Apple device, you can bring up the boot menu by pressing theOption key immediately after powering on the device.
For more information, see Apple’s knowledge base.
Cognition

Cognition

Cognition

From Wikipedia
In sciencecognition is the set of all mental[disambiguation needed] abilities and processes related to knowledgeattentionmemory and working memoryjudgement and evaluationreasoningand “computation“, problem solving and decision makingcomprehension and production of language, etc. Human cognition is conscious and unconscious, concrete or abstract, as well as intuitive (like knowledge of a language) and conceptual (like a model of a language). Cognitive processes use existing knowledge and generate new knowledge.
These processes are analyzed from different perspectives within different contexts, notably in the fields of linguisticsanesthesianeurosciencepsychiatrypsychologyeducation,philosophyanthropologybiologysystemics, and computer science.[1][page needed] These and other different approaches to the analysis of cognition are synthesised in the developing field ofcognitive science, a progressively autonomous academic discipline. Within psychology and philosophy, the concept of cognition is closely related to abstract concepts such as mind andintelligence. It encompasses the mental functionsmental processes (thoughts), and states of intelligent entities (humans, collaborative groups, human organizations, highly autonomous machines, and artificial intelligences).[2]
Thus, the term’s usage varies across disciplines; for example, in psychology and cognitive science, “cognition” usually refers to an information processing view of an individual’s psychologicalfunctions. It is also used in a branch of social psychology called social cognition to explain attitudesattribution, and group dynamics.[3] In cognitive psychology and cognitive engineering, cognition is typically assumed to be information processing in a participant’s or operator’s mind or brain.[2]
Cognition can in some specific and abstract sense also be artificial.[4]

Etymology[edit]

The word cognition comes from the Latin verb cognosco (con ‘with’ and gnōscō ‘know’) (itself a cognate of the Greek verb γι(γ)νώσκω gi(g)nόsko, meaning ‘I know, perceive’ (noun: γνώσιςgnόsis ‘knowledge’)) meaning ‘to conceptualize’ or ‘to recognize’.[5]

Origins[edit]

“Cognition” is a word that dates back to the 15th century when it meant “thinking and awareness.[6] “Attention to the cognitive process came about more than twenty-three centuries ago, beginning with Aristotle and his interest in the inner workings of the mind and how they affect the human experience. Aristotle focused on cognitive areas pertaining to memory, perception, and mental imagery. The Greek philosopher found great importance in ensuring that his studies were based on empirical evidence; scientific information that is gathered through thorough observation and conscientious experimentation.[7] Centuries later, as psychology became a burgeoning field of study in Europe and then gained a following in America, other scientists like Wilhelm Wundt, Herman Ebbinghaus, Mary Whiton Calkins, and William James, to name a few, would offer their contributions to the study of cognition.
Wilhelm Wundt (1832–1920) heavily emphasized the notion of what he called introspection: examining the inner feelings of an individual. With introspection, the subject had to be careful to describe his or her feelings in the most objective manner possible in order for Wundt to find the information scientific.[8][9] Though Wundt’s contributions are by no means minimal, modern psychologists find his methods to be quite subjective and choose to rely on more objective procedures of experimentation to make conclusions about the human cognitive process.
Herman Ebbinghaus (1850–1909) conducted cognitive studies that mainly examined the function and capacity of human memory. Ebbinghaus developed his own experiment in which he constructed over 2,000 syllables made out of nonexistent words, for instance EAS. He then examined his own personal ability to learn these non-words. He purposely chose non-words as opposed to real words to control for the influence of pre-existing experience on what the words might symbolize, thus enabling easier recollection of them.[8][10] Ebbinghaus observed and hypothesized a number of variables that may have affected his ability to learn and recall the non-words he created. One of the reasons, he concluded, was the amount of time between the presentation of the list of stimuli and the [clarification needed]. His work heavily influenced the study of serial position and its affect on memory, discussed in subsequent sections.
Mary Whiton Calkins (1863–1930) was an influential American female pioneer in the realm of psychology. Her work also focused on the human memory capacity. A common theory, called the Recency effect, can be attributed to the studies that she conducted.[11] The recency effect, also discussed in the subsequent experiment section, is the tendency for individuals to be able to accurately recollect the final items presented in a sequence of stimuli. Her theory is closely related to the aforementioned study and conclusion of the memory experiments conducted by Herman Ebbinghaus.[12]
William James (1842–1910) is another pivotal figure in the history of cognitive science. James was quite discontent with Wundt’s emphasis on introspection and Ebbinghaus’ use of nonsense stimuli. He instead chose to focus on the human learning experience in everyday life and its importance to the study of cognition. James’ major contribution was his textbook Principles of Psychology that preliminarily examines many aspects of cognition like perception, memory, reasoning, and attention to name a few.[12]

Genetics[edit]

The evolution of human brain and cognitive function is driven by different networking or feedback processes underlying genetic and environmental system.[13]

Psychology[edit]

Diagram

When the mind makes a generalization such as the concept oftree, it extracts similarities from numerous examples; the simplification enables higher-level thinking.

The sort of mental processes described as cognitive are largely influenced by research which has successfully used this paradigm in the past, likely starting with Thomas Aquinas, who divided the study of behavior into two broad categories: cognitive (how we know the world), and affective (how we understand the world via feelings and emotions)[disputed – discuss].[citation needed] Consequently, this description tends to apply to processes such asmemoryassociationconcept formationpattern recognitionlanguageattentionperceptionactionproblem solving and mental imagery.[14][15]Traditionally, emotion was not thought of as a cognitive process. This division is now regarded as largely artificial, and much research is currently being undertaken to examine the cognitive psychology of emotion; research also includes one’s awareness of one’s own strategies and methods of cognition called metacognition and includes metamemory.
Empirical research into cognition is usually scientific and quantitative, or involves creating models to describe or explain certain behaviors.
While few people would deny that cognitive processes are a function of the brain, a cognitive theory will not necessarily make reference to the brain or other biological process (compare neurocognitive). It may purely describe behavior in terms of information flow or function. Relatively recent fields of study such as cognitive science and neuropsychology aim to bridge this gap, using cognitive paradigms to understand how the brain implements these information-processing functions (see also cognitive neuroscience), or how pure information-processing systems (e.g., computers) can simulate cognition (see also artificial intelligence). The branch of psychology that studies brain injury to infer normal cognitive function is called cognitive neuropsychology. The links of cognition to evolutionary demands are studied through the investigation of animal cognition. And conversely, evolutionary-based perspectives can inform hypotheses about cognitive functional systems’ evolutionary psychology.
The theoretical school of thought derived from the cognitive approach is often called cognitivism.
The phenomenal success of the cognitive approach can be seen by its current dominance as the core model in contemporary psychology (usurpingbehaviorism in the late 1950s). Cognition is severely damaged in dementia.

Social process[edit]

For every individual, the social context in which he or she is embedded provides the symbols of his or her representation and linguistic expression. The human society sets the environment where the newborn will be socialized and develop his or her cognition. For example, face perception in human babies emerges by the age of two months: young children at a playground or swimming pool develop their social recognition by being exposed to multiple faces and associating the experiences to those faces. Education has the explicit task in society of developing cognition. Choices are made regarding the environment and permitted action that lead to a formed experience.
Language acquisition is an example of an emergent behavior. From a large systemic perspective, cognition is considered closely related to the social and human organization functioningand constrains. For example, the macro-choices made by the teachers influence the micro-choices made by students..

Piaget’s theory of cognitive development[edit]

For years, sociologists and psychologists have conducted studies on cognitive development or the construction of human thought or mental processes.
Jean Piaget was one of the most important and influential people in the field of Developmental Psychology. He believed that humans are unique in comparison to animals because we have the capacity to do “abstract symbolic reasoning.” His work can be compared to Lev VygotskySigmund Freud, and Erik Erikson who were also great contributors in the field of Developmental Psychology. Today, Piaget is known for studying the cognitive development in children. He studied his own three children and their intellectual development and came up with a theory that describes the stages children pass through during development.[16]
Stage Age or Period Description
Sensorimotor stage Infancy (0–2 years) Intelligence is present; motor activity but no symbols; knowledge is developing yet limited; knowledge is based on experiences/ interactions; mobility allows child to learn new things; some language skills are developed at the end of this stage. The goal is to develop object permanence; achieves basic understanding of causality, time, and space.
Pre-operational stage Toddler and Early Childhood (2–7 years) Symbols or language skills are present; memory and imagination are developed; nonreversible and nonlogical thinking; shows intuitive problem solving; begins to see relationships; grasps concept of conservation of numbers; egocentric thinking predominates.
Concrete operational stage Elementary and Early Adolescence (7–12 years) Logical and systematic form of intelligence; manipulation of symbols related to concrete objects; thinking is now characterized by reversibility and the ability to take the role of another; grasps concepts of the conservation of mass, length, weight, and volume; operational thinking predominates nonreversible and egocentric thinking
Formal operational stage Adolescence and Adulthood (12 years and on) Logical use of symbols related to abstract concepts; Acquires flexibility in thinking as well as the capacities for abstract thinking and mental hypothesis testing; can consider possible alternatives in complex reasoning and problem solving.[17]

Common experiments[edit]

Serial position
The serial position experiment is meant to test a theory of memory that states that when information is given in a serial manner, we tend to remember information in the beginning of the sequence, called the primacy effect, and information in the end of the sequence, called the recency effect. Consequently, information given in the middle of the sequence is typically forgotten, or not recalled as easily. This study predicts that the recency effect is stronger than the primacy effect because the information that is most recently learned is still in working memory when asked to be recalled. On the other hand, information that is learned first still has to go through a retrieval process. This experiment focuses on human memory processes.[18]
Word superiority
The word superiority experiment presents a subject with a word or a letter by itself for a brief period of time, i.e. 40ms, and they are then asked to recall the letter that was in a particular location in the word. By theory, the subject should be able to correctly recall the letter when it was presented in a word than when it was presented in isolation. This experiment focuses on human speech and language.[19]
Brown-Peterson
In the Brown-Peterson experiment, participants are briefly presented with a trigram and in one particular version of the experiment, they are then given a distractor task asking them to identify whether a sequence of words are in fact words, or non-words (due to being misspelled, etc.). After the distractor task, they are asked to recall the trigram which they were presented with before the distractor task. In theory, the longer the distractor task, the harder it will be for participants to correctly recall the trigram. This experiment focuses on human short-term memory.[20]
Memory span
During the memory span experiment, each subject is presented with a sequence of stimuli of the same kind; words depicting objects, numbers, letters that sound similar, and letters that sound dissimilar. After being presented with the stimuli, the subject is asked to recall the sequence of stimuli that they were given in the exact order in which they were given it. In one particular version of the experiment, if the subject recalled a list correctly, the list length increased by one for that type of material, and vice versa if it was recalled incorrectly. The theory is that people have a memory span of about seven items for numbers, the same for letters that sound dissimilar and short words. The memory span is projected to be shorter with letters that sound similar and longer words.[21]
Visual search
In one version of the visual search experiment, participants are presented with a window that displayed circles and squares scattered across it. The participant is to identify whether there is a green circle on the window. In the “featured” search, the subject is presented with several trial windows that have blue squares or circles and one green circle or no green circle in it at all. In the “conjunctive” search, the subject is presented with trial windows that have blue circles or green squares and a present or absent green circle whose presence the participant is asked to identify. What is expected is that in the feature searches, reaction time, that is the time it takes for a participant to identify whether a green circle is present or not, should not change as the number of distractors increases. Conjunctive searches where the target is absent should have a longer reaction time than the conjunctive searches where the target is present. The theory is that in feature searches, it is easy to spot the target or if it is absent because of the difference in color between the target and the distractors. In conjunctive searches where the target is absent, reaction time increases because the subject has to look at each shape to determine whether it is the target or not because some of the distractors if not all of them, are the same color as the target stimuli. Conjunctive searches where the target is present take less time because if the target is found, the search between each shape, stops.[22]
Knowledge representation
The semantic network of knowledge representation systems has been studied in various paradigms. One of the oldest is the leveling and sharpening of stories as they are repeated from memory studied by Bartlett. The semantic differential used factor analysis to determine the main meanings of words, finding that value or “goodness” of words is the first factor. More controlled experiments examine the categorical relationships of words in free recall. The hierarchical structure of words has been explicitly mapped in George Miller‘s Wordnet. More dynamic models of semantic networks have been created and tested with neural network experiments based on computational systems such as latent semantic analysis (LSA), Bayesian analysis, and multidimensional factor analysis. The semantics (meaning) of words is studied by all the disciplines of cognitive science.[citation needed]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. Jump up^ Von Eckardt, Barbara (1996). What is cognitive science?. Massachusetts: MIT Press.ISBN 9780262720236.
  2. Jump up to:a b Blomberg, O. (2011). “Concepts of cognition for cognitive engineering”. International Journal of Aviation Psychology 21 (1): 85–104. doi:10.1080/10508414.2011.537561.
  3. Jump up^ Sternberg, R. J., & Sternberg, K. (2009). Cognitive psychology (6th Ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.
  4. Jump up^ Boundless. Anatomy and Physiology. Boundless, 2013. p. 975.
  5. Jump up^ Stefano Franchi, Francesco Bianchini. “On The Historical Dynamics Of Cognitive Science: A View From The Periphery”. The Search for a Theory of Cognition: Early Mechanisms and New Ideas. Rodopi, 2011. p. XIV.
  6. Jump up^ Cognition: Theory and Practice by Russell Revlin
  7. Jump up^ Matlin, Margaret (2009). Cognition. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. p. 4.
  8. Jump up to:a b Fuchs, A. H.; Milar, K.J. (2003). “Psychology as a science”. Handbook of psychology 1 (The history of psychology): 1–26. doi:10.1002/0471264385.wei0101.
  9. Jump up^ Zangwill, O. L. (2004). The Oxford companion to the mind. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 951–952.
  10. Jump up^ Zangwill, O.L. (2004). The Oxford companion to the mind. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 276.
  11. Jump up^ Madigan, S.; O’Hara, R. (1992). “Short-term memory at the turn of the century: Mary Whiton Calkin’s memory research”. American Psychologist 47 (2): 170–174. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.47.2.170.
  12. Jump up to:a b Matlin, Margaret (2009). Cognition. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. p. 5.
  13. Jump up^ Fareed M, Afzal M. (2014) Estimating the inbreeding depression on cognitive behavior: A population based study of child cohort. PLoS ONE 9(10):e109585.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0109585 PMID 25313490
  14. Jump up^ Sensation & Perception, 5th ed. 1999, Coren, Ward & Enns, p. 9
  15. Jump up^ Cognitive Psychology, 5th ed. 1999, Best, John B., p. 15-17
  16. Jump up^ Cherry, Kendra. “Jean Piaget Biography”. The New York Times Company. Retrieved18 September 2012.
  17. Jump up^ Parke, R. D., & Gauvain, M. (2009). Child psychology: A contemporary viewpoint (7th Ed.). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
  18. Jump up^ Surprenant, A (2001). “Distinctiveness and serial position effects in total sequences”.Perception and Psychophysics 63 (4): 737–745. doi:10.3758/BF03194434PMID 11436742.
  19. Jump up^ Krueger, L. (1992). “The word-superiority effect and phonological recoding”. Memory & Cognition 20 (6): 685–694. doi:10.3758/BF03202718.
  20. Jump up^ Nairne, J.; Whiteman, H.; Kelley, M. (1999). “Short-term forgetting of order under conditions of reduced interference”. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology 52: 241–251. doi:10.1080/713755806.
  21. Jump up^ May, C.; Hasher, L.; Kane, M. (1999). “The role of interference in memory span”. Memory & Cognition 27 (5): 759–767. doi:10.3758/BF03198529.
  22. Jump up^ Wolfe, J.; Cave, K. & Franzel, S. (1989). “Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search”. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15 (3): 419–433. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.15.3.419.

Further reading[edit]

  • Coren, Stanley; Lawrence M. Ward; James T. Enns (1999). Sensation and PerceptionHarcourt Brace. p. 9. ISBN 0-470-00226-3.
  • Lycan, W.G., (ed.). (1999). Mind and Cognition: An Anthology, 2nd Edition. Malden, Mass: Blackwell Publishers, Inc.
  • Stanovich, Keith (2009). What Intelligence Tests Miss: The Psychology of Rational Thought. New Haven (CT): Yale University Press. ISBN 978-0-300-12385-2Lay summary (21 November 2010).

External links[edit]

Wikiversity has learning materials about Cognition
Wikibooks has a book on the topic of: Cognitive psychology
Look up cognition in Wiktionary, the free dictionary.